Cognitivism
In contrast to the behaviourist approach, McKenna (1995, pp
25) finds that cognitivists view learning as ‘an internal purposive process
concerned with thinking, perception, organisation and insight.’ Since there is
a change in the capability of the individual to respond to stimulus, it is not
possible to directly observe learning. Thinking involves the search for new
information as well as recall from the past experiences to gain understanding.
Since many theories adopt an information processing
framework (Cust 1995) Gagne’s work is referred to here as well. While
behaviourist approach is important in simple forms of learning, the thinking
process is felt to be of greater importance where complex human behaviour is
considered. Learning is ‘an active, constructivist process in which, learners
strive for understanding and competence on the basis of their personal
experience’ (Cust 1995, pp. 280 – 281). Therefore success relies on the
interaction of the learning and the knowledge required for the task as well as
prior knowledge of the learner. This means that the learner will be actively
and purposefully extracting and imposing meaning adding to already known
information. Since past knowledge and experiences are important, the concept of
perception, memory, and cognitive development are considered in the
understanding of the process of learning.
Development Theory
Piaget’s work provides an understanding of the way the
‘mental maps’ (also input in the constructivist view) are formed. He states
that a developing child builds cognitive structure or mental map to understand
and respond to environment. The cognitive structure will increase in
sophistication with development. There are four developmental stages :
- Sensorimotor stage (birth to 2 years) – through physical interaction with the environment the child will build set of concepts about reality and how it works. The interactions are either sensory or motor in nature, as the baby thinks through action.
- Preoperational stage (2 years to 7 years) – at this time of life the child still needs concrete physical stimulus to aid understanding as he is unable to conceptualise abstractly yet. Gross (1992) explains that the child’s world is still concrete and absolute and the child is influenced by how things look rather than by logical principles.
- Concrete Operations (7 years to 11 years) – the child begins to conceptualise and create logical structures, which explain the physical experience. Abstract problem solving is also possible during this stage.
- Formal Operations (11 years – 15 years) – the cognitive structure is more like that of an adult and conceptual reasoning is possible.
Figure 1
During all stages learning is taking place by using the
existing mental maps to understand the experience. If the experience is a
repetition then it is understood and assimilated into the mental make up. If it
is new it alters the mental map to accommodate the new experience. This allows
for building up more and more adequate cognitive structures. It must however be
kept in mind that the ages identified in each stage are only approximate ones
as children develop at different rates. Also the process of development is
continuous and not taking place in discrete segments. Gross (1992, pp. 741)
cites Piaget (1970) to clarify this point, ‘cognitive stages have a sequential
property, that is, they appear in a fixed order of succession because each one
of them is necessary for the formation of the following one.’
Gestalt Theory
This theory was put forward by a school of German
psychologists like Wertheimer, Koffka and Kohler (McKenna 1995). ‘Gestalt’ is a
German word meaning form, shape, figure or pattern (Child 1986). The
psychologists worked with chimpanzees. When faced with a problem such as how to
obtain food that was placed out of reach, the chimp did not tackle it by trial
and error as expected, but came to a sudden insightful solution. As soon as the
solution is perceived, the learner can proceed to use the insight.
The classical example of this is the insightful deduction by
Archemides when he identified that mass of an object is equal to the water it
displaces, when placed in water. He had all the related issues but was not just
able to perceive the solution. Then one day he sat in a bathtub full of water
and the water split over the sides. Suddenly he could see the solution. (Hiero
II requested that Archimedes find a method for determining whether a crown was
pure gold or alloyed with silver. When he stepped into a bath he realized that
a given weight of gold would displace less water than an equal weight of silver
(which is less dense than gold); at this point he shouted, "EUREKA"
(I have found it!). That is, he could
organise and integrate what he perceived into an over all pattern. From this
concept arises the phrase – the whole is greater than the sum of its parts.
Wertheimer (1912, cited by Childs 1981) stated that the
individual did not perceive the constituent elements of a phenomenon separately
but as a totality. He formulated the Law of Pragnanz to explain the four
aspects of perception :
- Similarity – people group phenomena by their familiar salient features and not by their differences.
- Proximity – people group phenomena by their closeness to each other instead of their distances.
- Continuity – objects are often perceived in relation to pattern or shape that they constitute in their totality.
- Closure – there is a tendency to complete an incomplete representation so that the whole is perceived rather than the incomplete parts.
Koffka believed that laws of perception could also be
regarded as the laws of learning (Quinn 2000). Since perception is deemed
important to learning it will be discussed in greater depth in the next section
of the book. Kohler (1947) indicated that solutions to problems appear to come
abruptly like a flash of insight. This happens because insight comes from
perception of relationship between the different factors and not as a response
to different stimuli. All learning cannot be insightful as it would imply that
insight is rooted in the past experiences or early socialisation.
Constructivism
This process describes learning as reflecting on experience
and construction of own understanding of the world around us. Learning is a
search for meaning and this search begins with the issue around which the
learner is actively trying to construct meaning. The focus of learning is on
primary concepts, as ‘parts’ must be understood in the context of the ‘whole’.
Each of us has developed our own mental model and rules (as suggested by
Gestalt theorists) to make sense of our experience. So learning is about
revising, reorganising and reinterpreting existing knowledge in order to
accommodate new information. (Spiro 1977 cited by Cust 1995)
This active and purposeful effort by the learner will make
the information more meaningful and lead to deep learning. The active
participation of the leaner in the process of learning is possible due to
motivation. Motivation is also seen as another explanation into the process of
learning because, humanist theorists also discuss it. It will be discussed in
greater depth in the next section of this book.
Another concept important to the constructivism is
metacognition, which relates to knowledge and regulation of cognition. It
involves higher order behaviour such as problem solving, critical thinking and
decision making. This comes about from reflecting on experience, learning from
it, and to apply the learning to a new situation (Boud et al 1985). Some of
these ideas, for example that of active experience, reflection, learning and
application of that learning are the basis of humanistic theories discussed
later in this section.
Discovery Learning
The concept of discovery learning has often been referred to
as a part of the educational philosophy by philosophers like Rousseau, and
Dewey, ‘there is an intimate and necessary relation between the processes of
actual experience and education’ (Dewey 1938). It also enjoys the support of
learning theorists/psychologists, Piaget and Bruner where the objective is to
lead the child to discover for himself (Bruner 1962). Discovery learning occurs
when individuals must use their mental processes to figure out the meaning of
something for themselves. Discovery learning takes place most notably in
problem solving situations where the learner draws on his own experience and
prior knowledge to discover the truths that are to be learned. It is a
personal, internal, constructivist learning environment. This allows the
learner to be a constructionist, to organize what he is encountering in a
manner to discover regularity and relatedness, as well as to avoid the kind of
information drift that fails to keep account of the uses to which information
might have to be put.
Discovery learning has a variety of operational definitions.
The definitions show its existence on a continuum. At one end lies discovery
learning in its purest form. The tools and information needed to solve a
problem or learn a concept are provided and the learner "makes sense"
of them. At half way is discovery learning as experimentation with some
extrinsic intervention - clues, coaching, a framework to help learners get to a
reasonable conclusion. At the other end of the continuum, lies the expository
teaching model of discovery learning where the learner "discovers"
what the teacher decides he is to discover using a process prescribed by the
teacher.
In all instances of discovery learning the teacher’s
intervention determines how "pure" is the operational definition of
discovery learning. The level of teacher intervention recommended maybe an
indicator of the philosophy of education of the teacher/facilitator. The
current trend of supporting the "teacher as
facilitator/learner-centred" model of instruction and its constructivist
philosophical orientation could find a home on this continuum, as well as the
teacher-centred model and its objectivist philosophical orientation.
The advantage of discovery learning is that questioning and
solving problems without expecting someone to give the answer, enables the
learner to develop self-confidence to handle problems in this area, leading to
further motivation to learn. Active learning supports the belief that knowledge
can be constructed by the learner rather than received from a higher authority.
Discovery learning is based on the assumption that education
is a process rather than a set of facts.
- Active learning puts the responsibility on the learner. When placed in the position of having to figure out a problem, the leaner much more likely to take charge of own learning.
- Learner is more attentive.
- The activities focus attention on the key ideas or techniques that are being examined.
- Active involvement forces the leaner to construct a response and this results in processing of information deeper than mere memorization.
- Discovery learning provides an opportunity to get early feedback on own understanding: gaps in understanding cannot be ignored.
- Active learning results in "episodic memory," a deeper memory specific to an event so that if you cannot at first remember the idea or technique you can reconstruct it from the event.
- Discovery learning can be more motivating, incorporating the pleasures of solving puzzles and controlling an environment.
From beginning to end, discovery learning programs keep the
experience of gaining insights and commitment, interesting and relevant.
Several steps in the creation of a discovery learning design make this work:
- Attract and Intrigue - Immediate visual and auditory stimuli attract attention and set an exciting pace and tone. Right from the start, learners are surprised, engaged, excited and involved in the process of learning.
- Jump-start - Fast-paced exercises get learners started in the process of discovery, by providing just enough initial information and directions for them to confidently move ahead.
- Brain-churn - Small groups work on their own. They "play", think, analyse, question, and occasionally compete. Team members rely on each other and learn together.
- Discover and Conclude - The light bulbs start burning brightly as learners grasp the big picture, "discover" new insights, knowledge and concepts and ultimately draw conclusions that are pulled together into an overall context or concept.
- Bridge and Connect - Finally the learners connect the learning to the real world. Activities that bridge this new learning to personal follow-up action on-the-job, make the learning deep, truly relevant and long-lasting. (Paradigm Learning 2000)
Hi Mriga,
ReplyDeleteI would include a citation so that anyone wanting to use your work (and I know they will) can easily cite you as the source. Of course you know APA/MLA formats are both widely used.
yes i have kept the reference list out so people cannot plagiarise.. its easy to look up and author and date
ReplyDelete